Summary
Awards
In the News
Progress
Research Basis
Publications
Photos
Videos
People
|
Abstract
This document summarizes two types of published research
underlying Project LISTEN’s automated Reading Tutor. Intervention studies measured the Reading
Tutor’s effectiveness. Other research,
others’ as well as our own, served to guide its development. The cited Project LISTEN publications can
be downloaded from www.cs.cmu.edu/~listen
except where precluded by copyright or not yet in print.
Acknowledgements
The
work described here was supported in part by NSF
under ITR/IERI Grant
REC-0326153, by the Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of
Education through Grant R305A080628 to Carnegie Mellon University, and
by the Heinz Endowments. Any opinions,
findings, and conclusions or recommendations expressed in this material are
those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the views of the
National Science Foundation or the Heinz Endowments.
Special thanks to the co-principal investigators who helped formulate the
IERI proposals from which portions of this document are excerpted, especially
reading expert Professor Rollanda O’Connor.
1.
Summary of intervention
studies
Speech-recognition-based,
computer-guided oral reading has demonstrated usability, user acceptance,
assistive effectiveness, and even pre- to post-test gains [Cole et
al., 1999; Mostow et
al., 1994; Nix et al.,
1998; Russell et
al., 1996; Williams,
2002; Williams et
al., 2000]
– but the proof of the pudding is whether it significantly increases
learning gains over gains that children make otherwise. Even with barely 20 minutes of use per day,
successive versions of the Reading Tutor have produced substantially higher
comprehension gains than current practices in controlled studies lasting
several months. To ensure that results were due to the Reading Tutor
intervention, we compared different treatments within the same classrooms and
randomized treatment assignment, stratifying by pretest scores within class. We used valid and reliable measures [Woodcock,
1998]
to measure gains from pre- to post-test.
We computed effect size as the difference in gains between the
Reading Tutor and current practice, divided by the average standard deviation
in gains of the two groups. Effect
sizes for passage comprehension were substantial compared to other studies [NRP, 2000]: 0.60 for 63 students in grades 2, 4, and 5
at a low-income urban school [Mostow and Aist, 2001; Mostow et al., 2003b]; 0.48 for 66 third graders at a
lower-middle class urban school [Aist et al., 2001; Mostow et al., 2001; Mostow et al., 2003a]; and 0.66 for 52 first graders at
two suburban Blue Ribbon Schools of Excellence [Mostow et al., 2002b; Mostow and Beck, under
revision].
1.1. Pilot study (1996-97)
The Reading Tutor achieved
dramatic results in the first pilot study of extended use long enough to
demonstrate significant learning.
During the 1996-97 school year, a pilot group of low-reading third
graders used the Reading Tutor one at a time in a small office under the
individual supervision of a school aide.
According to school-administered pre- and post-tests, six third
graders who started almost three years below grade level averaged two years
of progress in under eight months use [Aist and
Mostow, 1997].
1.2. Within-classroom comparison (1998)
In spring 1998, we did our first controlled study of
the Reading Tutor in classroom settings at Fort Pitt Elementary [Mostow et al., 2003b]. All 72 students
in 3 classrooms (grades 2, 4, and 5) that had not previously used the Reading
Tutor were independently pre-tested on the Word Attack, Word Identification, and Passage
Comprehension subtests of the Woodcock Reading Mastery
Test [Woodcock,
1987]. We split each class into 3 matched treatment groups
– Reading Tutor, commercial reading software, or
regular classroom activities, including other software use. We assigned students to treatments randomly,
matched within classroom by pretest scores.
Even
though the study lasted only 4 months, and actual usage was a fraction of the
planned daily 20-25 minutes, students who used the 1998 version of the
Reading Tutor significantly outgained their matched classmates in
comprehension (effect size .60, p = .002), progressing
faster than their national cohort. (No
other differences were significant, and commercial software fell in
between.) As the principal said,
“these children were closing the gap.”
In
1999-2000, we evaluated the new, mixed story choice version of the Reading
Tutor at a second school in a lower-middle class community near Pittsburgh. This year-long study of 131 second and third
graders in 12 classrooms compared three daily 20-minute treatments. (a) 58
students in 6 classrooms used the 1999-2000 version of the Reading
Tutor. Students took daily turns using
one shared Reading Tutor in their classroom while the rest of their class
received regular instruction. (b) 34
students in the other 6 classrooms were pulled out daily for one-on-one
tutoring by certified teachers. To
control for materials, the human tutors used the same set of stories as the
Reading Tutor. (c) 39 students served
as in-classroom controls, receiving regular instruction without
tutoring. We pre- and post-tested
students in word identification, word attack, word comprehension, passage
comprehension, and fluency.
To our
surprise, human tutors beat the Reading Tutor only in Word Attack (effect
size .55). Third graders in both the
computer- and human-tutored conditions outgained the control group in Word
Comprehension (effect sizes of .56 and .72, respectively) and Passage
Comprehension (effect sizes of .48 and .55, respectively) [Aist et
al., 2001; Mostow et
al., 2001]. No other differences in gains were
significant.
1.4. Equal-time comparison to Sustained
Silent Reading
(2000-2001)
According
to the National Reading Panel, “the amount of gain attributable to reading
alone should be the baseline comparison against which the efficacy of
instructional procedures is tested. If an instructional method does better
than reading alone, it would be safe to conclude that method works” [NRP, 2000,
Ch. 3, p. 27]. A 7-month
study of 178 students in grades 1-4 at two Blue Ribbon Schools of Excellence
compared two treatments, each provided in daily 20-minute sessions. 88 students did Sustained Silent Reading
(SSR) as already implemented in their classrooms (including teacher
read-aloud in grade 1 until students were ready for independent reading
practice). 90 students in 10-computer
labs used the 2000-2001 version of Project LISTEN’s Reading Tutor. The Reading Tutor group significantly
outgained their statistically matched SSR classmates in phonemic awareness,
rapid letter naming, word identification, word comprehension, passage
comprehension, fluency, and spelling – especially in grade 1, where effect
sizes for between-treatment differences in gains ranged from .20 to .72 [Mostow et
al., 2002b].
1.5. Effectiveness for English language
learners [this section added 6/6/05 and updated 8/11/11]
2004 marked
the first independent, third-party, controlled evaluation of the Reading
Tutor [Poulsen,
2004]. This two-month pilot study included 34
second through fourth grade Hispanic students from four bilingual education
classrooms. The study compared the efficacy of the 2004 version of the
Project LISTEN Reading Tutor against the standard practice of Sustained
Silent Reading (SSR). This study was undertaken to obtain some initial
indication as to whether the tutor would also be effective within a
population of English language learners.
The study
employed a crossover design where each participant spent one month in each of
the treatment conditions. The experimental treatment consisted of 25
minutes per day using the Reading Tutor within a small pullout lab
setting. Students in the control treatment remained in the classroom
where they participated in established reading instruction activities.
Dependent variables consisted of the school district’s curriculum based
measures for fluency, sight word recognition, and comprehension.
The Reading
Tutor group outgained the control group in every measure during both halves
of the crossover experiment. Within-subject results from a paired
T-Test indicate that these gains were significant for one sight word measure
(p = .056) and both fluency measures (p < .001). Effect sizes were
0.55 for timed sight words, a robust 1.16 for total fluency and an even
larger 1.27 for fluency controlled for word accuracy. These dramatic
results observed during a one-month treatment indicate that this technology
may have much to offer English language learners.
Two
additional groups of Canadian researchers conducted independent evaluations of
the Reading Tutor with English language learners and as of June 2005 are
analyzing the data.
A 10-week
study by Kenneth Reeder, Margaret Early, Maureen Kendrick, Jon Shapiro, and
Jane Wakefield at the University of British Columbia [CTV, 2006;
D’Silva et al., 2005; Reeder et al., 2004; Reeder et al., 2005; Reeder et al., 2007; Reeder et al., 2009]
involved 77 students from five Vancouver elementary schools, grades 2-6 (ages
7-12 years). Their home languages were
Hindi (14), Mandarin (21), Spanish (21), and English (21: 11 using the
Reading Tutor, and 10 in a human tutoring program). Gains by the Reading Tutor group matched
gains by the human tutoring group on most reading measures, and interviews
showed favorable affect impact by the Reading Tutor.
A 12-week
study by Esther Geva and Todd Cunningham at the University of Toronto [Cunningham,
2006; Cunningham
and Geva, 2005] involved 104 ESL students
in grades 4-6 at eight schools. The
study compared three treatments: the
Reading Tutor; Kurzweil 3000, which reads aloud to the student and provides
vocabulary support; and regular ESL classroom instruction. Analysis of data from 77 students in Grades
4-6 found pre- to posttest gains on some measures of language and literacy skill,
with no significant differences among conditions, but did not measure oral
reading fluency.
An 18-week
crossover study in Accra, Ghana, [Korsah et
al., 2010] provided the Reading Tutor as a supplemental
intervention for 89 children in 3 schools varying in affluence. It found treatment effect sizes of over 1
standard deviation (considered large) for fluency gains at the two poorer
schools and for spelling gains at one of them, but no significant differences
between treatments at the most affluent school.
A 10-week
crossover study in Bangalore, India, [Weber and
Bali, 2010] focused on 62 low-income elementary school
students at 3 schools. This population
had little or no exposure to English outside of school. Overall, they averaged significantly higher
gains in oral reading fluency (but not in spelling) over the 5 weeks during
which they used the Reading Tutor than over the other 5 weeks.
2.
Summary of underlying
research
Why does the
Reading Tutor improve comprehension?
Theoretically, students who recognize words effortlessly can devote
more attention to comprehension [LaBerge and
Samuels, 1974], and the relationship
between rate of oral reading and reading comprehension is strong through the
elementary years [Pinnel et
al., 1995]. The
cognitive load imposed by word identification before it has become a mentally
automatic process consumes limited mental resources, such as attention and
short term memory, needed to comprehend the sentence and its relationship to
the surrounding context [Perfetti,
1992].
However,
decoding practice by itself does not necessarily improve fluency or
comprehension. Some studies found that
teaching children to recognize isolated words quickly gave no advantage in
reading comprehension [Fleischer
et al., 1979], or that comprehension did not improve unless
readers recognized the words nearly as fast in context as in lists [Levy et
al., 1997]. Thus
fluency makes a unique contribution to comprehension over that made by word
identification [Ehri and
McCormick, 1998; O'Connor et
al., 2002; Shankweiler
et al., 1999].
Guided oral
reading provides opportunities to practice word identification and
comprehension in context. There is
ample evidence that one of the major differences between good and poor
readers is the amount of time they spend reading. Poor readers are unlikely to practice on
their own. Students who need the most
practice spend the least amount of time actually reading [Allington,
1977]. How time is spent reading matters
too [Mostow et
al., 2002a]. Poor
readers tend to reread the same easy stories over and over [Aist, 2002a]. Modifying the Reading Tutor to take turns
picking stories exposed students to more new vocabulary than they saw when
they chose the stories [Aist, 2002a;
Aist, 2002b; Aist and Mostow, 2003; Mostow et al., 2003b].
The Reading
Tutor aims for the zone of proximal development [Doolittle,
1997]
by dynamically updating its estimate of the student’s reading level, and
picking stories accordingly – which are somewhat harder than students choose
when it is their turn [Mostow et
al., 2003a].
The Reading Tutor scaffolds
key processes in reading – and tests its own scaffolding. Scaffolding provides information at the
“teachable moments” when it is needed.
For example, explicit vocabulary instruction is important but
time-consuming [Beck et
al., 2002].
Explaining unfamiliar words and concepts in context can remediate
deficits in vocabulary and background knowledge [Elley, 1989], so we added support for
vocabulary acquisition by presenting short “factoids” – comparisons to other
words [Aist, 2001b;
Aist, 2002a]. An automated experiment embedded in the
Reading Tutor tested the effectiveness of reading a factoid just before a new
word in a story, compared to simply encountering the word in context without
a factoid. The outcome variable was
performance on a multiple-choice question, presented the next day the student
used the Reading Tutor. Analysis of
over 3,000 randomized trials showed that factoids helped on rare,
single-sense words, and that they helped third graders more than second
graders [Aist, 2000;
Aist, 2001a; Aist, 2001b].
By acquiring predictive models of the effects of tutorial actions, embedded
experiments can inform a decision-theoretic approach to tutoring [Beck, 2001;
Beck, 2002; Beck and Woolf, 2000; Beck and Woolf, 2001; Beck et al., 2000; Murray et
al., revisions under review].
The zone of
proximal development depends on tutorial scaffolding as well as on student
proficiency [Murray and
Arroyo, 2002], so the Reading Tutor lets the student read as
much as possible, but helps as much as necessary. It provides spoken and graphical assistance
when it notices the student click for help, hesitate, get stuck, skip a word,
make a mistake, or encounter a word likely to be misread [Mostow and
Aist, 1999]. Its
“visual speech” [Massaro,
1998]
uses talking-mouth videoclips of phonemes to scaffold phonemic
awareness. The Reading Tutor assists word
identification by previewing new words [Mostow, to
appear] and reading hard words aloud. Its word attack hints include rhyming and
sounding out. It supports vocabulary
acquisition by explaining new words [Aist, 2001b;
Aist, 2002a; Mostow et al., 2003c]. It scaffolds comprehension by reading hard
sentences aloud and by asking questions [NRP, 2000] – “cloze” items [Mostow et
al., 2002c] and generic “who-what-where” questions, which
at first appeared to boost comprehension of nearby sentences in an embedded
experiment [Beck et
al., 2003]. The
Reading Tutor bolsters motivation by listening attentively, “backchanneling” [Aist and
Mostow, 1999], giving encouragement [Aist et
al., 2002], and praising good or improved performance [Mostow and
Aist, 1999]. By reducing frustration [Betts, 1946] and making a wide range of
authentic, engaging text cognitively accessible to the child, scaffolding
helps address the motivational issues of confidence, challenge, curiosity,
and control pivotal to effective tutoring [Lepper and
Chabay, 1988; Lepper et
al., 1993]. Poor
readers’ listening comprehension is far above their independent reading level
[Curtis,
1980; Spache,
1981],
so reading hard words and sentences to them reduces frustration and repairs
comprehension failures caused by lack of automaticity in word identification.
One approach
to improving automaticity is repeated reading, in which students read a
passage or page of text until their reading rate increases by a given amount,
usually 25% or more [Samuels,
1979]. A recent review of the repeated reading
literature [Meyer and
Felton, 1999] recommended that poor readers practice
building fluency for 10-20 minutes per day over a long duration, engage in reading
aloud, and use text at their instructional level. However, improving word
recognition accuracy and comprehension can require assistance to remediate
errors [McCoy and
Pany, 1986; Young et
al., 1996] – which requires listening to the student read
aloud.
References (download
Project LISTEN publications at www.cs.cmu.edu/~listen)
AIST, G. 2000. Helping
Children Learn Vocabulary during Computer-Assisted Oral Reading. Ph.D.
dissertation Thesis, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA.
AIST, G. 2001a. Factoids:
Automatically constructing and administering vocabulary assistance and
assessment. In Artificial Intelligence
in Education: AI-ED in the Wired and
Wireless Future. Tenth Artificial Intelligence in Education (AI-ED)
Conference, J.D. MOORE, C.L. REDFIELD and W.L. JOHNSON, Eds. Amsterdam: IOS Press, San Antonio, Texas, 234-245.
AIST, G. 2001b. Towards
automatic glossarization:
Automatically constructing and administering vocabulary assistance
factoids and multiple-choice assessment. International
Journal of Artificial Intelligence in Education 12, 212-231.
AIST, G. 2002a. Helping
Children Learn Vocabulary During Computer-Assisted Oral Reading. Educational Technology and Society 5(2), http://ifets.ieee.org/periodical/vol_2_2002/aist.html.
AIST, G. 2002b. Helping
Children Learn Vocabulary during Computer-Assisted Oral Reading: A Dissertation Summary [Poster presented as
a Distinguished Finalist for the Outstanding Dissertation of the Year Award]. In 47th Annual Convention of the International Reading Association,
San Francisco, CA, April 29, 2002b.
AIST, G., KORT, B., REILLY,
R., MOSTOW, J. and PICARD, R. 2002. Experimentally Augmenting an Intelligent
Tutoring System with Human-Supplied Capabilities: Adding Human-Provided Emotional Scaffolding
to an Automated Reading Tutor that Listens.
In Proceedings of the Fourth IEEE
International Conference on Multimodal Interfaces (ICMI 2002),
Pittsburgh, PA, October 14-16, 2002, IEEE, 483-490.
AIST, G. and MOSTOW, J. 1997.
When Speech Input is Not an Afterthought: A Reading Tutor that Listens. In Workshop on Perceptual User Interfaces, Banff, Canada, October,
1997.
AIST, G. and MOSTOW, J. 1999.
Measuring the Effects of Backchanneling in Computerized Oral Reading Tutoring. In Proceedings of the ESCA Workshop on Prosody and Dialog,
Eindhoven, Netherlands, September, 1999.
AIST, G. and MOSTOW, J. 2003.
Faster, better task choice in a reading tutor that listens. In Speech Technology for Language Learning,
V.M. HOLLAND and F.N. FISHER, Eds. Swets & Zeitlinger Publishers, Lisse,
The Netherlands.
AIST, G., MOSTOW, J., TOBIN,
B., BURKHEAD, P., CORBETT, A., CUNEO, A., JUNKER, B. and SKLAR, M.B. 2001.
Computer-assisted oral reading helps third graders learn vocabulary better
than a classroom control – about as well as one-on-one human-assisted oral
reading. In Artificial Intelligence in
Education: AI-ED in the Wired and
Wireless Future. Tenth Artificial Intelligence in Education (AI-ED)
Conference, J.D. MOORE, C.L. REDFIELD and W.L. JOHNSON, Eds. Amsterdam: IOS Press, San Antonio, Texas, 267-277.
ALLINGTON, R. 1977. If they
don't read much, how they ever gonna get good? Journal of Reading 21,
57-61.
BECK, I.L., MCKEOWN, M.G. and
KUCAN, L. 2002. Bringing Words to
Life: Robust Vocabulary Instruction.
Guilford, NY.
BECK, J.E. 2001. ADVISOR:
A Machine-Learning Architecture for Intelligent Tutor Construction.
Ph.D. Thesis, University of Massachusetts Amherst, Amherst, 168 pp.
BECK, J.E. 2002. Directing
Development Effort with Simulated Students.
In Proceedings of the Sixth
International Conference on Intelligent Tutoring Systems, Biarritz,
France, 2002, 851-860.
BECK, J.E., MOSTOW, J.,
CUNEO, A. and BEY, J. 2003. Can automated questioning help children's reading
comprehension? In Proceedings of the
Tenth International Conference on Artificial Intelligence in Education
(AIED2003), Sydney, Australia, July 20-24, 2003, 380-382.
BECK, J.E. and WOOLF, B.P.
2000. High-level Student Modeling with Machine Learning. In Fifth International
Conference on Intelligent Tutoring Systems, 2000, 584-593.
BECK, J.E. and WOOLF, B.P.
2001. Using Rollouts to induce a policy from a user model. In Eighth International
Conference on User Modeling, 2001,
210-212.
BECK, J.E., WOOLF, B.P. and
BEAL, C.R. 2000. ADVISOR: A machine learning architecture for intelligent
tutor construction. In Proceedings of the Seventeenth National
Conference on Artificial Intelligence, Austin, Texas, 2000, 552-557.
BETTS, E.A. 1946. Foundations of Reading Instruction.
American Book Company, New York.
COLE, R., MASSARO, D.W.,
VILLIERS, J.D., RUNDLE, B., SHOBAKI, K., WOUTERS, J., COHEN, M., BESKOW, J.,
STONE, P., CONNORS, P., TARACHOW, A. and SOLCHER, D. 1999. New tools for
interactive speech and language training: Using animated conversational
agents in the classrooms of profoundly deaf children. In ESCA/SOCRATES Workshop
on Method and Tool Innovations for Speech Science Education, London, UK,
April, 1999.
CTV 2006. Interview with Ken
Reeder on the Vancouver Reading Tutor Project (3m28s). Used by permission, CTV News, Canada.
CUNNINGHAM, T. 2006. The Effect of Reading Remediation Software
on the Language and Literacy Skill Development of ESL Students. Master's
thesis Thesis, University of Toronto, Toronto, Canada.
CUNNINGHAM, T. and GEVA, E.
2005. The effects of reading technologies on literacy development of ESL
students [poster presentation]. In Twelfth Annual Meeting of the Society for
the Scientific Study of Reading, Toronto, June 24, 2005.
CURTIS, M.E. 1980.
Development of components of reading skill. Journal of Educational Psychology 72(5), 656-669.
D’SILVA, R., HONG, L.,
LENTERS, K., EARLY, M., KENDRICK, M., REEDER, K., SHAPIRO, J. and WAKEFIELD,
J. 2005. LISTENing to diverse learners: The effectiveness and appropriateness
of a computer-based reading tutor for young multilingual learners [poster
presentation]. In Fifth Faculty of Education Research Day,
The University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, May, 2005.
DOOLITTLE, P.E. 1997.
Vygotsky's Zone of Proximal Development as a Theoretical Foundation for
Cooperative Learning. Journal on
Excellence in College Teaching 8(1),
83-103.
EHRI, L. and MCCORMICK 1998.
Phases of word learning: Implications for instruction with delayed and
disabled readers. Reading and Writing
Quarterly 14, 135-164.
ELLEY, W.B. 1989. Vocabulary
acquisition from listening to stories. Reading
Research Quarterly 24, 174-187.
FLEISCHER, L.S., JENKINS,
J.R. and PANY, D. 1979. Effects on poor readers' comprehension of training in
rapid decoding. Reading Research
Quarterly 14, 30-48.
KORSAH, G.A., MOSTOW, J.,
DIAS, M.B., SWEET, T.M., BELOUSOV, S.M., DIAS, M.F. and GONG, H. 2010.
Improving Child Literacy in Africa: Experiments with an Automated Reading
Tutor. Information Technologies and
International Development 6(2),
1-19.
LABERGE, D. and SAMUELS, J. 1974.
Toward a theory of automatic information processing in reading. Cognitive Psychology 6, 293-323.
LEPPER, M.R. and CHABAY, R.W.
1988. Socializing the intelligent tutor: Bringing empathy to computer tutors.
In Learning issues for intelligent
tutoring systems, H. MANDL and A. LESGOLD, Eds. Springer, NY, 242-257.
LEPPER, M.R., WOOLVERTON, M.,
MUMME, D.L. and GURTNER, J. 1993. Motivational Techniques of Expert Human
Tutors: Lessons for the Design of
Computer-Based Tutors. In Computers as
Cognitive Tools, S.P. LAJOIE and S.J. DERRY, Eds. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ,
75-105.
LEVY, B., ABELLO, B. and
LYSYNCHUK, L. 1997. Transfer from word training to reading in context: Gains
in reading fluency and comprehension. Learning
Disabilities Quarterly 20,
173-188.
MASSARO, D. 1998. Perceiving Talking Faces. MIT Press,
Cambridge, MA.
MCCOY, K. and PANY, D. 1986.
Summary and analysis of oral reading corrective feedback research. The Reading Teacher 39, 548-555.
MEYER, M.S. and FELTON, R.H.
1999. Repeated reading to enhance fluency: Old approaches and new directions.
Annals of Dyslexia 49, 283-306.
MOSTOW, J. to appear.
Evaluation purposes, excuses, and methods:
Experience from a Reading Tutor that listens. In Facilitating Constructivist Literacy Environments Through Technology,
C.K. KINZER and L. VERHOEVEN, Eds. Erlbaum, Mahway, NJ.
MOSTOW, J. and AIST, G. 1999.
Giving help and praise in a reading tutor with imperfect listening -- because
automated speech recognition means never being able to say you're certain. CALICO Journal 16(3), 407-424.
MOSTOW, J. and AIST, G. 2001.
Evaluating tutors that listen: An overview of Project LISTEN. In Smart Machines in Education, K. FORBUS
and P. FELTOVICH, Eds. MIT/AAAI Press, Menlo Park, CA, 169-234.
MOSTOW, J., AIST, G., BECK,
J., CHALASANI, R., CUNEO, A., JIA, P. and KADARU, K. 2002a. A la recherche du
temps perdu, or as time goes by: Where
does the time go in a Reading Tutor that listens? In Proceedings of the Sixth International Conference on Intelligent
Tutoring Systems (ITS'2002), Biarritz, France, June 5-7, 2002a, S.A.
CERRI, G. GOUARDÈRES and F. PARAGUAÇU, Eds. Springer, 320-329.
MOSTOW, J., AIST, G., BEY,
J., BURKHEAD, P., CUNEO, A., JUNKER, B., ROSSBACH, S., TOBIN, B., VALERI, J.
and WILSON, S. 2002b. Independent practice versus computer-guided oral
reading: Equal-time comparison of sustained silent reading to an automated
reading tutor that listens. In Ninth Annual Meeting of the Society for
the Scientific Study of Reading, Chicago, Illinois, June 27-30, 2002b, J.
WILLIAMS, Ed.
MOSTOW, J., AIST, G.,
BURKHEAD, P., CORBETT, A., CUNEO, A., EITELMAN, S., HUANG, C., JUNKER, B.,
PLATZ, C., SKLAR, M.B. and TOBIN, B. 2001. A controlled evaluation of
computer- versus human-assisted oral reading. In Artificial Intelligence in Education:
AI-ED in the Wired and Wireless Future. Tenth Artificial
Intelligence in Education (AI-ED) Conference, J.D. MOORE, C.L. REDFIELD and
W.L. JOHNSON, Eds. Amsterdam: IOS
Press, San Antonio, Texas, 586-588.
MOSTOW, J., AIST, G.,
BURKHEAD, P., CORBETT, A., CUNEO, A., EITELMAN, S., HUANG, C., JUNKER, B.,
SKLAR, M.B. and TOBIN, B. 2003a. Evaluation of an automated Reading Tutor
that listens: Comparison to human
tutoring and classroom instruction. Journal
of Educational Computing Research 29(1),
61-117.
MOSTOW, J., AIST, G., HUANG,
C., JUNKER, B., KENNEDY, R., LAN, H., LATIMER, D., O'CONNOR, R., TASSONE, R.,
TOBIN, B. and WIERMAN, A. 2003b. 4-Month Evaluation of a Learner-controlled
Reading Tutor that Listens. In Speech
Technology for Language Learning, V.M. HOLLAND and F.N. FISHER, Eds.
Swets & Zeitlinger Publishers, Lisse, The Netherlands.
MOSTOW, J. and BECK, J. under
revision. Computer-Guided Oral Reading versus Independent Practice: Comparison of Sustained Silent Reading to
an Automated Reading Tutor that Listens.
MOSTOW, J., BECK, J., BEY,
J., CUNEO, A., SISON, J. and TOBIN, B. 2003c. An Embedded Experiment to
Evaluate the Effectiveness of Vocabulary Previews in an Automated Reading
Tutor. In Tenth Annual Meeting of the Society for Scientific Studies of Reading,
Boulder, CO, June 12-15, 2003c.
MOSTOW, J., ROTH, S.F.,
HAUPTMANN, A.G. and KANE, M. 1994. A prototype reading coach that listens
[AAAI-94 Outstanding Paper]. In Proceedings of the Twelfth National
Conference on Artificial Intelligence, Seattle, WA, August, 1994,
American Association for Artificial Intelligence, 785-792.
MOSTOW, J., TOBIN, B. and
CUNEO, A. 2002c. Automated comprehension assessment in a reading tutor. In Proceedings of the ITS 2002 Workshop on Creating Valid Diagnostic
Assessments, San Sebastian, Spain, June 3, 2002c, 52-63.
MURRAY, R.C., VANLEHN, K. and
MOSTOW, J. revisions under review. Looking Ahead to Select Tutorial Actions:
A Decision-Theoretic Approach. International
Journal of Artificial Intelligence in Education.
MURRAY, T. and ARROYO, I.
2002. Towards Measuring and Maintaining the Zone of Proximal Development in
Adaptive Instructional Systems. In Sixth International Conference on
Intelligent Tutoring Systems,
2002, 749-758.
NIX, D., FAIRWEATHER, P. and
ADAMS, B. 1998. Speech Recognition, Children, and Reading. In CHI 98, 1998, 245-246.
NRP 2000. Report of the
National Reading Panel. Teaching children to read: An evidence-based assessment of the
scientific research literature on reading and its implications for reading
instruction. 00-4769, National Institute of Child Health & Human
Development. At www.nichd.nih.gov/publications/nrppubskey.cfm, Washington, DC.
O'CONNOR, R.E., BELL, K.M.,
HARTY, K.R., LARKIN, L.K., SACKOR, S. and ZIGMOND, N. 2002. Teaching Reading
to Poor Readers in the Intermediate Grades: A Comparison of Text Difficulty. Journal of Educational Psychology 94, 474-485.
PERFETTI, C.A. 1992. The
representation problem in reading acquisition. In Reading Acquisition, P.B. GOUGH, L.C. EHRI and R. TREIMAN, Eds.
Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, 145-174.
PINNEL, G.S., PIKULSKI, J.J.,
WIXSON, K.K., CAMPBELL, J.R., GOUGH, P.B. and BEATTY, A.S. 1995. Listening to
children read aloud, Office of Educational Research and Improvement, U.S.
Department of Education, Washington, DC.
POULSEN, R. 2004. Tutoring Bilingual Students With an
Automated Reading Tutor That Listens:
Results of a Two-Month Pilot Study. Master's thesis Thesis, DePaul
University, Chicago, IL.
REEDER, K., EARLY, M.,
KENDRICK, M., SHAPIRO, J., D'SILVA, R., HONG, L., LAU, K., LENTERS, K. and
WAKEFIELD, J. 2004. Listening to Diverse Learners: The Effectiveness and
Appropriateness of a Computer-based Reading Tutor for Young Canadian Language
Learners (INTERIM REPORT, YEAR ONE), Department of Language & Literacy
Education, The University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC.
REEDER, K., EARLY, M.,
KENDRICK, M., SHAPIRO, J. and WAKEFIELD, J. 2005. The Role of L1 in Young
Multilingual Readers' Success With a Computer-Based Reading Tutor. In Fifth International Symposium on Bilingualism, Barcelona, Spain,
April, 2005.
REEDER, K., SHAPIRO, J. and
WAKEFIELD, J. 2007. The effectiveness of speech recognition technology in
promoting reading proficiency and attitudes for Canadian immigrant children. In 15th European Conference on Reading, Humboldt University, Berlin,
August 5-8, 2007, Jointly organised by International Development in Europe
Committee of the International Reading Association (IDEC) and the Deutsche
Gesellschaft für Lesen und Schreiben (German Association of Reading and
Writing).
REEDER, K., SHAPIRO, J. and
WAKEFIELD, J. 2009. A computer based reading tutor for young English language
learners: recent research on proficiency gains and affective response. In 16th European Conference on Reading and 1st Ibero-American Forum on
Literacies, University of Minho, Campus de Gualtar, Braga, Portugal, July
19-22, 2009, A.-S. SELIN and M.D.L. DIONÍSIO, Eds. International Development
in Europe Committee of the International Reading Association (IDEC).
RUSSELL, M., BROWN, C.,
SKILLING, A., SERIES, R., WALLACE, J., BONHAM, B. and BARKER, P. 1996.
Applications of automatic speech recognition to speech and language
development in young children. In Proceedings of the Fourth International
Conference on Spoken Language Processing, Philadelphia PA, 1996.
SAMUELS, S.J. 1979. The
method of repeated readings. The
Reading Teacher 32, 403-408.
SHANKWEILER, D., LUNDQUIST,
E., KATZ, L., STUEBING, K.K., FLETCHER, J.M., BRADY, S., FOWLER, A., DREYER,
L., MARCHIONE, K.E., SHAYWITZ, S.E. and SHAYWITZ, B.A. 1999. Comprehension
and decoding: Patterns of association in children with reading difficulties. Scientific Studies of Reading 3, 69-94.
SPACHE, G.D. 1981. Diagnostic Reading Scales.
McGraw-Hill, Monterey, CA.
WEBER, F. and BALI, K. 2010.
Enhancing ESL Education in India with a Reading Tutor that Listens,
Proceedings of the First ACM Symposium on Computing for Development ACM,
London, United Kingdom, 20:1-9.
WILLIAMS, S.M. 2002. Speech
recognition technology and the assessment of beginning readers. In Technology and assessment: Thinking ahead: Proceedings of a workshop, NRC, Ed.
National Academy Press, Board on Testing and Assessment, Center for
Education, Division of Behavioral and Social Sciences and Education. Washington, DC, 40-49.
WILLIAMS, S.M., NIX, D. and
FAIRWEATHER, P. 2000. Using Speech Recognition Technology to Enhance Literacy
Instruction for Emerging Readers.
In Fourth International Conference of
the Learning Sciences, 2000, B.
FISHMAN and S. O'CONNNOR-DIVELBISS, Eds. Erlbaum, 115-120.
WOODCOCK, R.W. 1987. Woodcock Reading Mastery Tests-Revised.
American Guidance Service, Circle Pines, MN.
WOODCOCK, R.W. 1998. Woodcock Reading Mastery Tests - Revised
(WRMT-R/NU). American Guidance Service, Circle Pines, Minnesota.
YOUNG, A.R., BOWERS, P.G. and
MACKINNON, G.E. 1996. Effects of prosodic modeling and repeated reading on
poor readers' fluency and comprehension. Applied
Psycholinguistics 17, 59-84.
|